This is my lit review as of June 1, 2002:
A review of
taste perception emphasizing metallic attribute perception
by John Smythe
To
understand perception of oral stimuli, one should consider the physiology of
the sensory organs in the tongue. Plattig (1988) reports that the taste pore
is an opening in the papillae approximately 2 µm in diameter. From the
taste pore, microvilli of 0.1-0.2 µm diameter and 1-2 µm length
extend into the oral cavity. After the diffusion of taste-active compounds through
the saliva, these microvilli are believed to bear chemical receptors that bind
taste molecules in the initial stages of taste perception. These receptor cells
bind taste molecules and start the cascading sequence of events that leads to
perception. Basal cells exist, as well, with the apparent purpose of regenerating
taste cells.
The terminal branchings of myelinated nerve fibers
are known as telodendria. The diameter of these fibers ranges from 1-6 µm.
These, combined with unmyelinated fibers of 0.3-1 µm in diameter, enervate
the taste buds. The unmyelinated fibers are thought to carry information of
the vegetative nerve system to the taste cells in an efferent system. In contrast,
the telondria are believed to transmit the gustatory afferent information to
the central nervous system. These signals are carried to the brain via the seventh
and the ninth cranial nerves. The seventh cranial nerve, N. VII, is known as
the facialis nerve and is responsible for stimuli on the anterior two thirds
of the tongue. The ninth cranial nerve, N. IX, is known as the glossopharyngeus
nerve, and is responsible for signals on the posterior one-third of the tongue.
There also exist pharyngeal and oesophageal taste buds, but these are generally
connected to the tenth cranial nerve, N. X, known as the vagus nerve (Plattig,
1988). Unfortunately, as of yet there exists no reported experimental evidence
investigating the taste response of individual human nerve fibers in relation
to either the peripheral or central nervous system (Delwiche, 1996).
Boudreau (1986) offers suggestions based on animal
studies regarding what type of taste information is carried by the various cranial
nerves. He suggests that the trigeminal nerve, N. V, carries astringent, pungent,
and temperature information as part of the free nerve ending system. He further
suggests that in the taste bud system, the facial nerve, N. VII, carries information
regarding sweet, bitter, salty, insipid, and sour stimuli, and that the glossopharyngeal
nerve (IX) carries information regarding sweet, bitter, metallic and umami compounds.
The role that the vagus nerve, N. X, has in taste perception is not well understood
(Boudreau, 1986; Yanagisawa, 1998).
Henning (1916) proposed a tetrahedron of the four
basic tastes of salty, sweet, sour, and bitter, could define the taste realm
if the basic tastes were placed at the corners of the tetrahedron. This work
assumed that other taste sensations were nothing more than mixtures of primaries
that were located on the edges or surfaces of the tetrahedron (Bartoshuk, 1978,
Delwiche, 1996). However, studies on food have revealed that these four sensations
alone are not sufficient to describe the perception of various foods (Boudreau
et al, 1979; Boudreau, 1983). One may demonstrate that certain stimuli still
fall outside Henning's (1916) tetrahedral model of the basic tastes by eliminating
olfactory stimulation using a technique of forcing a gentle stream of air into
the nostrils (Mozell et al, 1969). The same results may be achieved by using
totally anosmic subjects (Mozell et al, 1969; Schiffman, 1980), demonstrating
that the four basic tastes in conjunction with olfaction is inadequate to fully
describe the flavor of foods. Anosmics, or those who have lost the ability to
smell, often describe their food as tasteless or bland. Testing their perception
of sweet, sour, bitter, and salty stimuli reveals that their ability to perceive
these basic tastes has not been altered (Plattig, 1988). Although the flavor
of the food constituents clearly has been modified, it is not the taste but
rather the volatile aromas that migrate from the mouth through the choanae (posterior
or pharyngeal nares) to the olfactory receptors that are not perceived in these
cases (Piggott, 1988).
Schiffman and Erickson (1971) attempted to expand
the taste space through the use of multidimensional scaling. However, this work
quickly met with criticism endemic to the use of multidimensional scaling (McBurney
and Gent, 1979). They argued that multidimensional scaling did not lead to the
discovery of new taste dimensions, but rather served to enhance understanding
of the relationships among already established taste dimensions. They further
argued that scaling of a small subset of stimuli often gives fundamentally different
results than scaling the entire set of stimuli. As such, they argued studies
using multidimensional scaling to date were incomplete and inadequate to properly
model the complete taste space and results were often confounded by the specific
stimuli selected for the study.
Tastes such as metallic, soapy, alkaline, and others, have been suggested, but
their modes of perception are not well understood, and as such, they have not
been universally accepted in the taste literature. Metallic taste is often reported
following tastings of metal salts. A metallic sensation is also reported after
tasting certain formed metals, though according to their surface electric potentials,
these formed metals might actually produce what has been classified as an electric
taste (Plattig, 1988). Cathodal DC stimulation has been shown to cause alkaline
taste (soapy), while anodal stimulation leads to the perception of a sour taste
(Plattig, 1988). This suggests that these tastes may both be linked to electric
taste, though this leaves the exact nature of the metallic taste somewhat of
a mystery. The reported tastes of metallic, soapy, or sour in carbonated beverages
may also be due to altered ion balance within the taste buds, which may disrupt
the normal pattern interactions of receptor cells (Roper, 1989), and lead to
what are sometimes known as "phantom tastes," often associated with
neurological damage or pathology (El-Deiry & McCabe, 1990). This contrasts
with the research of Skramlik (1926), who reviewed work that suggested that
the alkaline and metallic tastes result exclusively from olfactory stimuli and
disappear when such stimuli are excluded.
No studies to date have investigated the taste
response of individual human nerve fibers; as such, no physiological system
has been described as an analog for the human metallic sensation. Boudreau (1986)
has suggested the PGsalt system of the rat petrosal ganglion as a possible analog
since it responds to solutions with metallic cations without the Na+ and Li+
specificity shown by rat GGsalt units. Caution must be used in applying such
analogs to the human taste system. Studies of basic physiological measurements
reveal that notable difference exist among mammals (Pritchard, 1991). Comparisons
of humans to great apes and monkeys reveal fundamental differences in central
nervous system gustatory anatomies (Pritchard, 1991), and differences among
different mammalian orders in anatomy is well documented (Smith-Swintosky et
al, 1991). Even among members of the same species, there is evidence that taste
transduction mechanisms for a specific tastant may have great variability (Kinnamon
and Cummings, 1992). These findings beckon restraint when attempting to apply
physiological models developed from animal experiments to humans (Delwiche,
1996).
In human studies anesthetizing the chorda tympani
branch of the facial nerve via the ear canal (Yanagisawa et al, 1992), 40% of
subjects reported taste phantoms that appeared at the contralateral rear of
the tongue when stimulated by non-tastant containing solutions. Phantoms most
often were described as salty, sweet, sour, bitter, or metallic. By anesthetizing
the location of the reported taste, one could eliminate the taste phantoms.
Norgren and Pfaffman (1975) have shown that topical anesthesia eliminates spontaneous
nerve activity, which led Bartoshuk (1993) to suggest that anesthetizing the
chorda tympani allows the brain to perceive the basal activity of the glossopharyneal
nerve, thus explaining the source of the phantom tastes.
Bartoshuk (1993) discusses one subject who, recovering
from a mastoidectomy that likely damaged or transected the chorda tympani, complained
of a constant intense salty taste. Rinsing with a topical anesthetic comprised
of 0.5 % dyclonine and 0.5% diphenhydramine in 0.9% saline led to a reduction
of intense salty taste, presumably by eliminating the glossopharyngeal input.
Early studies by Bartoshuk and Kveton (1991) suggest that neural responses from
the chorda tympani nerve are usually inhibited by the neural responses of the
glossopharyngeal nerve, but later research suggests that patients with localized
damage in taste system often do not report a decrease in their ability to perceive
tastes (Bartoshuk, 1993).
Perception of metallic attributes by metallic stimuli in non-food systems
Metallic has been interchangeably used to describe
taste, mouthfeel, and aroma, depending on the context. Gonázlez Viñas
and Salvador (1998) compared method of limits ASTM E-679 for threshold determination
(1991) and Lundahl's threshold determination method based on scales using iron
(II) sulfate heptahydrate as a standard for metallic tastes in the range of
1.3 - 8.0 mg/L, as suggested by International Standard ISO 3972 (1991). Böröcz-Szabó
(1980) notes that iron (II) sulfate, also known as ferrous sulfate or FeSO4,
is the iron compound most advantageously utilized by humans. Using a set of
21 panelists, Gonázlez Viñas and Salvador showed judges were less
able to correctly identify metallic tastes than other taste categories of sweet,
salty, acid, bitter, and umami. This may be a remnant of panelist familiarity
with these other taste qualities in contrast to their familiarity with metallic
stimuli. Regardless, the authors showed using ASTM E-679 methodology that sensitivities
for FeSO4 in water ranged from 2.26 - 9.56 mg/L, with a group mean threshold
that was 7.90 mg/L. This was contrasted with Lundahl's method, which found all
individual and group detection thresholds to exceed 8.00 g/L. They compared
this with the suggested ISO range of 1.3 - 8.0 mg/L, arguing that the international
standard ISO 3972 concentrations were lower than they should be for use of Lundahl's
method with untrained panels. This may be due to the fact that, as Böröcz-Szabó
(1980) notes, in tap water, both ferrous and ferric salts form red precipitates
soon after dissolution, though in deionized water, only ferric salts produced
a red precipitate. Ferric salts of FeCl3 were colorless to concentrations of
1 mg dm-3, while Fe2(SO4)3 solutions were colorless to 2 mg dm-3.
Metallic taste was reported as a major defect
in the water supplied by water utilities, based on a taste and odor questionnaire
with 377 responses from the United States and Canada (Suffet et al, 1996). The
authors used a taste and odor wheel (Mallevialle and Suffet, 1987) that classified
metallic as a "mouthfeel" sensation rather than a taste. Although
sour water was the most reported taste problem, metallic taste was the second
most frequently reported taste problem, followed by chlorination. Linssen et
al (1991) applied factor analysis of tainted water in pouches of aluminum lined
with low-density polyethylene to data from a panel of 48 assessors. Results
reveal that metallic ratings were significantly different from control experiments,
suggesting the pouches influenced the flavor of the water.
The work of Nakagawa (1972) may justify the high
incidence of reported off-metallic flavor in water sources (Japanese article
- getting translated - insert interpreted regarding specific thresholds here).
They showed that mutlivalent metallic salts were shown to form soluble complexes
with proteins in water that lead to extreme levels of astringency. Presumably,
if these multivalent compounds were present in the water, they could bind with
salivary proteins and lead to the reported off-metallic flavors noted in the
work of Suffet et al (1996) and Linssen et al (1991).
Research by Roy (1992) shows that with saccharins,
the usual bitter, metallic taste is removed when a carbon atom is strategically
replaced by a sulfur or an oxygen atom. This may be significant when one considers
the AH-B-X model of sweetness reception, though the implications of this fact
are not fully understood.
Exposure to heavy metals and toxins has also been
shown to lead to metallic tastes. Reports (FDA Web Document B, 2002) discuss
metallic taste as a symptom of copper and lead poisoning with latency periods
of less than one hour, as well as a symptom of ciguatera poisoning with a 1-6
hour latency period. Barceloux (1999) reports zinc oxide as the most common
cause of metal fume fever, of which metallic taste is a symptom. This study
reports that seafood, meat, dairy products, nuts, legumes, and whole grains
contain relatively high concentrations of zinc. Mahaffey et al (1975) reports
that meats, poultry, and fish are considered to contain an average of about
25 mg Zn/kg.
Studies (Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease
Registry, 1997) show the ambient concentration of zinc in the air is generally
less than 1 microgram per cubic meter. Zinc chloride, zinc oxide, zinc stearate,
and zinc sulfate are commonly used food additive zinc salts that have legally
have generally recognized as safe, or GRAS, status. Zinc does not accumulate
in the body, with 70-80% eliminated fecally, and 15-25% eliminated through sweat
and urine. Zinc oxide fumes lead to the onset of metal fume fever within 4-10
hours of exposure, though metal fume fever may also occur following exposure
to aluminum, antimony, copper, iron, magnesium, manganese, and nickel fumes.
Studies (International Labour Office, 1983) show
that occupational long-term exposure in steel workers who handle tellurium pellets
often leads to reports of metallic tastes, as well as oft reported accounts
of strong garlic odors. Müller et al (1989) reports a study of the toxic
effects of tellurium reported for a 37 year old non-occupationally exposed woman
with tellurium intoxication. She experienced nausea and vomiting in conjunction
with a metallic taste in the mouth and a sharp garlic odor in her breath, sweat,
and excrement.
Kachru et al (1989) report chronic lead poisoning
among nine adults and nine children attributable to lead fumes and lead oxide
dust produced during the extraction of gold and silver from jewelers waste.
Pre-examination interviews showed five of seven male silver jewelery workers
reported sweet metallic tastes in their mouths. Heamatological examination and
urinalysis showed significant increases in blood lead, zinc protoporphyrin,
urinary excretion of lead and aminolevulinic acid, with associated decreases
in blood aminolevulinc acid dehydratase activity.
Perception of metallic attributes in non-animal flesh food systems
Yau and Huang (2000) conducted sensory tests on
membrane processed water used in Oolong tea using descriptive analysis and preference
tests among 15 trained panelists. This experiment used 12 g Oolong tea stem
extracted by 600g water at 100o C for 5 minutes to serve as the metallic aroma
standard. This same sample, when stirred for 20h with a magnetic stir bar, also
served as an oxidized aroma standard. The stems of the Oolong tea before extraction
served as the roasty aroma standard. It is unclear how the standards were justified
for this experiment. Regardless, when using unprocessed, nanofiltrated, and
reverse osmosis treated water, both oxidized and metallic aromas showed significant
increases with each of the respective treatments. The unprocessed tea sample,
showing the lowest scores for oxidized and metallic aromas, showed the lowest
hedonic scores among the samples. This is a rare case where the presence of
a metallic note does not inversely correlate with hedonic liking. However, this
study does not consider metallic aromas a key attribute of aroma; PCA of the
data considers metallic aroma as one of the least influential aspects of the
descriptive analysis, while oxidized aroma is a descriptive attribute noted
in PC1. The authors postulate that the higher presence of metallic aromas in
the reverse osmosis tea samples compared to those using nanofiltrated water
may be due to the presence of monovalent ions, though this blanket consideration
is applied to many attributes beyond metallic aroma and is not fully investigated.
In contrast to the prior study, Kumasawa and Masuda
(1999) used aroma extract dilution analysis to detect odorants of the volatile
fraction of a Japanese green tea (Sen-cha) to find odor-active peaks by GC/MS
and/or multidimensional GC/MS. They report (Z)-1,5-octadien-3-one as having
one of the highest flavor dilution values, FD = 1000, justifying it as one of
the most important odorants of Sen-cha. Guth and Grosh (1993) in prior work
had suggested that this was one of the 3 most important odorants in this tea,
which differs in production methods from Chinese green tea. This compound is
noted in both studies as having a characteristic metallic odor.
There exists evidence suggesting that iron
content can negatively affect the taste of teas. Chakraborty and Baruah (1971)
reported that excess iron in water produces a "brassy, harsh and metallic
taste" in cup-infusion tea, arguing that iron content of more than 2 ppm
is detrimental to tea quality. Unfortunately, no correlations were shown in
the work of Guth and Grosh (1993), Kumasawa and Masuda (1999), or Yau and Huang
(2000) relating the indirect effects of iron content to the levels of metallic
odorants in tea.
During informal discussions at the Eighth International
Colloquium on the Chemistry of Coffee in 1977, two experienced coffee tasters
reported that black coffee brews occasionally had a rather unpleasant astringent
taste that was often accompanied by a metallic taste that was quite distinct
from bitterness. Clifford and Ohiokpehai (1983) have explored these reports
in follow-up studies that focused on preliminary organoleptic evaluation of
dicaffeoylquinic acid, chosen for its similarity to galloylquinic acids used
in astringency research. Results suggest that this compound cannot be adequately
be described by the term "astringent," though it does present an easily
detected and lingering metallic taste in water and coffee in the range of 50-100
mg/L.
Böröcz-Szabó (1980) performed
a broad study that classified foodstuffs based on the concentrations at which
ferrous iron concentrations affected perception in the log10 schemes of those
requiring concentrations of 1000, 100, and 10 mg/L to be detected. The first
group contains fruit juices, the second group contains wines and liqueur, and
the third and most sensitive group contains beer and milk. At high concentrations
in fruit juices, bitter tastes were reported in addition to metallic flavor,
and salty notes were reported above 1500 mg/L. In wines, ferrous salts were
postulated to liberate hydrogen sulfide, the unpleasant smell of which likely
masked the flavor effects of the iron salts and minimized the identification
of metallic notes. Both ferric and ferrous salts caused substantial changes
in beer, both in loss of CO2 and in increases in turbidity. For milk products,
both ferric and ferrous salts imparted not only a metallic taste, but also led
to panelist reporting that the milk smelled stomach turning or rancid. Some
individuals reported the spiked milk samples smelled or tasted paper-like. The
counter-anions present in these studies had little effect on sensory impact.
Böröcz-Szabó notes that the taste of deionized water is unpleasant,
which makes it difficult to distinguish it from ferrous solutions because of
the inherent difficulty distinguishing between two unpleasant tastes. Reports
from panelists reveal lingering astringent, metallic sensations remained in
their mouth for a long time after testing. One may hypothesize that the presence
of lipids milk and iso-alpha-acids in beer leave them particularly prone to
degradation by both ferric and ferrous salts, while fruit juices, which are
high in antioxidant potential, can mask such contaminants to a much greater
extent.
Gains and Thomson (1990) employed free-choice
profiling using 19 consumers testing 17 canned lagers and analyzed their results
by general Procrustes analysis. Their results show that PC1 was responsible
for 43% of the variance and contrasted thin mouthfeel to rich, full, viscous,
syrupy, thick mouthfeel, sweet, syrupy flavor, strong, and alcoholic flavor.
PC2 was responsible for 8% of the variance and contrasted good head retention,
bitter flavor, and yeasty malty flavor against long aftertaste, metallic, sour
taste, and syrupy mouthfeel. The authors suggest that most important discriminator
of different beers and lagers are mouthfeel terms, while in this study metallic
notes were not major contributors to flavor.
Gautschi and Schmid (1997) report using a panel
of 5-7 experienced creative panelists that the diketopiperazine (DKP) cyclo(Val-Pro)
found in beer has a "lingering, metallic, salty taste at 10 ppm" and
found that increased levels of DKP's correlated with darker levels of malt.
De Graaf et al (1990) found that when diketopiperazines are combined with theobromine,
they result in a pronounced bitter metallic taste. Bonvehí and Coll (2000)
extended these findings to DKP levels in roasted cocoa powder, showing that
a trained panel of 10 judges considered levels of 80 mg kg-1 unacceptable under
all roasting conditions tested. At roasting temperatures of 145o C for 3 or
10 min, judges gauged the product as unacceptable, at levels as low as 74.2
DKP mg kg-1 cocoa powder, whereas the product was gauged as acceptable at both
125 and 135o C for the same roasting times with DKP levels not exceeding 72.3
mg DKP kg-1 cocoa powder. Furthermore, adding reducing sugars to the cocoa powder
lead to reduced levels of DKP in conditions that had previously yielded unsatisfactory
levels, and supporting the claim of increased levels of DKPs with more intense
kilning regimes.
Lindner et al (1997) evaluated a dihydrochalcone
sweetener, CH-401 in foods. When added to chocolate and vanilla parfait (100
mL milk, 4 egg yolks, 500 mL sweet cream), judges rated samples less well in
hedonic tests. The development of metallic after-taste was reported as the source
of the decrease in hedonic ratings. Subjects also reported a metallic aftertaste
in yellow cream (500 mL milk, 35 g flour, 15 g cream-powder, 3 eggs), and both
cooked and cold buttercreams were identified as having strong metallic tastes.
The CH401 content in these fives samples was 12.20, 12.0, 4.5, 102.6, and 102.6
mg per 100 g of samples, respectively. The authors report that in the case of
products containing egg-yolk and fats, the sweetening capacity is slight, but
the resulting metallic taste is distinct.
Other compounds have been shown to correlate to
metallic taste. Blouin and Vinnet (1985) report that phospholipid portion of
cottonseed flour is responsible for metallic taste. DuPont et al (1994) observed
a correlation between bitter and metallic and between these tastes and total
alkaloid content of slurries of milled lupin seed in water. Metallic tastes
were considered weak tastes in these samples by these judges, when compared
to other tastes that were evaluated. Bitter and green, and beany tastes were
the strongest tastes. No relationship was observed with tannin content and metallic
taste.
Poretta (1985) reports that by PCA, citric acid
is most closely related to metallic taste among compounds examined for canned
whole tomatoes. However, this study only tested various acids and sugars, as
well as pectins, protopectins, pectic acid, ethanol, and NaCl. No analysis of
any metallic compounds, or any compounds believed to elicit a metallic flavor,
was explicitly performed. Mahadeviah et al (1983) report that both soldered
and welded cans packed with different products retained good vacuums, but organoleptic
evaluations of potatoes stored in brine at 37o C report metallic tastes.
Davies (1936) found that iron present in milk
in the form of lactacte expressed a metallic flavor at concentrations of 15
ppm. Reports from El Dadek et al (1972) suggest that in pickled kareish cheese,
low quality cheeses are often reported as having firm texture or sharp acidic
tastes that are usually accompanied by metallic flavors. No sensory analysis
was reported in these studies to back such claims. Degouy (1993) showed by using
Simplesse 100, a whey by-product of Gouda manufacturing, a low-fat cheese was
made and subjected to an eight person sensory panel. It was reported that after
15 weeks, no metallic or bitter flavors occurred. Work by Rice and McMahon (1998)
compared control Mozzarella cheese to iron fortified Mozzarella cheese and found
that fortification with 25 or 50 mg of either FeCl3, iron chelated to whey protein,
or iron chelated to casein proteins, per kg of cheese; showed increases in oxidized,
metallic, and off-flavors. Judges who were trained to detect oxidized and metallic
flavors were more sensitive than the TBA assay in detecting differences in oxidation
flavors in cheese, suggesting that the TBA assay does not sufficiently model
these flavors in cheese. The authors suggest that oxidative and metallic flavors
of the iron fortified cheeses were less than expected, considering the role
of iron in lipid perodixation. When the Mozzarella cheeses were cooked on pizzas,
no differences were perceptible by the consumer panel.
Heiler and Schieberle (1997) state that it is
well known in the dairy industry that due to rapid off-flavor development, the
shelf-life of sour cream buttermilk is much shorter than that of fermented buttermilk.
The typical off-note developed during storage is often described as metallic
(Frank, 1984). Sour cream buttermilk contains 40% fat before fermentation, whereas
the so-called fermented buttermilk is usually derived from sweet-cream buttermilk
of 1% milkfat that is treated with butter starter cultures after separation
of butterfat. Using comparative aroma extraction dilution analysis applied to
flavor extracts prepared from fresh fermented sweet-cream buttermilk and a stored
sour-cream buttermilk exhibiting intense metallic flavors, Heiler and Schieberle
(1997) noted that metallic smelling odorants (E,Z)-2,6-nonadienol and 4,5-epoxy-(E)-2-decenal,
as well as the mothball smelling odorant 3-methylindole were the most important
odorant contributors to metallic defects in buttermilk. The (E,Z)-2,6-nonadienol
was reported as the key odorant responsible, which reportedly has been found
in several plant materials. Hatanaka et al (1975) suggest that this compound
may be formed in cucumbers from an enzymic reaction starting from alpha-linolenic
acid as a precursor.
Buettner and Schieberle (2001) quantified odor active compounds in fresh, hand
squeezed White Marsh seedless grapefruit juice using stable isotope dilution
assays and calculation of odor activity values. In this study, they showed that
trans-4,5-epoxy-(E)-dec-2-enal, noted as offering a metallic odor in the previously
mentioned studies, was among the most potent odorants. Prior work by Buettner
and Schieberle (1999) had identified the presence and nature of odorant using
GC/Olfactometry of serial dilutions, or Aroma Extract Dilution Analysis. These
results found concentration in fresh hand-squeezed grapefruit juice to be 0.0031
mg/kg. Rychlik et al (1998) suggest odor threshold of these compounds in air
is in the range of 0.0006-0.0025 bg/L, revealing the extreme olfactory sensitivity
in humans to this compound. Shrader and Johnson (1934) report that as little
as 5 ppm iron or copper can cause off-flavors in orange juice, though this is
not clearly identified as a metallic defect.
Trans-4,5-epoxy-(E)-nonenal and trans-4,5-epoxy-(E)-2-decenal
appear again in aroma studies by Stephan and Steinhart (1999). By performing
aroma extract dilution and GC-sniff techniques, these compounds are reported
as metallic contributors in standardized, hydrolyzed, and oil free commercial
soybean lecithins. Modified CHARM (combined hedonic and response measurements)
analysis rated trans-4,5-epoxy-(E)-decenal with a high CHARM values of 701,
907, and 649 for standardized, hydrolyzed, and oil free commercial soybean lecithins,
whereas the nonenal form rated CHARM values as <3 in all three samples, showing
lower CHARM values than any other 61 odorants studied. Flavor dilution values
for the decenal forms were 1024, 1024, and 512 versus values of 32, 16, and
4 for the nonenal for standardized, hydrolyzed, and oil free soybean lecithins.
Metallic is rated as a taste in the actual studies, but regarded in this study
by the authors as a retronasal sensation that increases through hydrolysis of
lecithins. The sensation decreases through acetone deoiling.
Volatile flavor compounds of miniature beefsteakplants
(Mosla dianthere Maxim) were analyzed through GC/MS/O (Kim et al, 2001). The
results report that (Z)-3-hexenol is an important aroma-active compound, with
a grassy/leafy/metallic aroma. In chemical contrast, Buettner and Schieberle
(2001) report (Z)-3-hexanal as important to the aroma of grapefruit juice, offering
only green/grassy notes. The concentration of (Z)-3-hexenol in miniature beefsteakplants
is reported as 4.2 ppm with an aroma threshold in water reported by Buttery
et al (1971) of 0.070 ppm. Hatakanaka (1996) reports this to be biosynthesized
in green leaves via lipoxygenase and hydroperoxide lyase-mediated lipid oxidation.
Perception of metallic attributes in animal flesh food systems
Extensive work has been performed analyzing metallic
notes in animal flesh foods, as well in the previously mentioned non-animal
flesh foods. Seafood, chicken, pork, and beef have all identified metallic off-flavors
in their products. In each case, this is considered a defect. Refsgaard et al
(1998) note that the most pronounced sensory changes during frozen storage of
salmon are increased intensity of train oil taste, bitterness, and metal taste,
which result not from volatile oxidation products, but instead from compounds
of low volatility. Increased levels of free fatty acid during frozen storage
are reported for several species of fish (Shewfelt, 1981). Refsgaard et al (2000)
demonstrated that polyunsaturated acids added to fresh salmon mince at levels
found in frozen stored salmon induced similar sensory phenomena as those found
in salmon after frozen storage. This suggests that enzymatic hydrolysis of neutral
lipids plays key role in sensory deterioration of salmon in frozen storage.
Taste assessment studies showed that palmitoleic acid, linoleic acid, eicosapentaeonic
acid, and docosahexaenoic acid show high intensity of train oil, bitter, and
metal taste. It is interesting to note, however, that the addition of the free
fatty acids did not affect the odor of the fish. They suggest that only small
amounts of oxidation products from the fatty acids are formed after addition
of the fatty acids and chopping of the salmon meat. As such, the total concentrations
of such by-products do not exceed the odor thresholds of these products in meat
model systems.
Lovell (1983) described major off-flavors in fish
and included the term metallic. A descriptive analysis study performed by Johnsen
et al (1987) using 18 people experienced in tasting catfish rated metallic as
an important flavor phenomena, but listed metallic as the chemical feeling factor
on the tongue, rather than a taste, and described it as a "flat" sensation
associated with iron and copper solutions. Occasional reports of metallic flavored
seafood cocktails have been given from time to time. These may result from biochemical
reactions, microbial spoilage, or accidental contamination by manmade products.
Whitfield et al (1982) have shown that the garlic-metallic off-flavors in the
royal red prawn (Hymenopenaeus sibogae) are likely due to bis-(methylthio)-methane,
oct-1-en-3-ol, and (5Z)-octa-1,5-dien-3-ol. Studies show that these off-flavor
compounds originate in the heads of royal red prawn, but upon cooking the compounds
diffuse into the edible portions of the meat (Davidson, 1984). Hanson
and Duckworth (1982) report that when pure carbon dioxide is used in controlled
atmosphere packaging on fish, marked drip loss occurs, accompanied by the development
of a metallic/acidic taste. The same is not reported to occur in red meat packaging.
Metallic has long been described as an immediate
off-taste/odor developed after irradiation (Morgan and Siu, 1957). Studies by
Risvik (1986) using descriptive analysis of irradiated beef and bacon were performed
using 10 trained panelists. Unfortunately, no chemical analysis was performed
to determine which components form the characteristic metal tastes. Other than
reporting samples as tasting metallic, subjects reported a prickling sensation
on the tongue that lasted for several hours after assessment. In Risvik's study,
metallic taste increases correlated with decreased juiciness, color saturation,
and increased off-taste, sweetness, and rancidity. Other studies on beef have
listed metallic as a descriptor (Berry et al, 1980; MacLeod and Coppock, 1978;
Persson et al, 1973).
Buscailon et al (1994) studied hams processed by dry curing for either 179 or
273 days and submitted them to chemical and sensory analysis. A 12 member trained
panel rated intensity and persistence of metallic aroma, again defined as the
result of retronasal olfactory perception. No differences were noted in between
the aged samples for total intensity or persistence of metal aroma, and the
authors fail to identify the compounds responsible for the source of metallic
aromas. A chemical analysis using GC/MS is presented, but reports many of the
compounds as unidentified or unknown.
Jeremiah et al (1996) analyzed 80 cured and smoked
hams for fat, sodium, potassium, chloride, and nitrite content. Hams were subjected
to a highly trained flavor profile panel. Each flavor note was reportedly described
along with the order of appearance of each note. In this study, fat content
was correlated to metallic aftertaste. The authors fail to identify specific
compounds responsible for the metallic aftertaste.
Heymann et al (1990) evaluated pork roasts cooked
to various endpoints by sensory and chemical analyses. A trained ten person
panel evaluated samples and specifically rated metallic flavor, defined as "The
degree of metallic 'sensation' perceived." Increased endpoint temperatures
were reported as associated with decreased metallic flavor. Canonical variate
analysis correlated increased endpoint temperatures with increased brown color
and decreased pink color, likely related to the conversion of oxymyoglobin,
metmyoglobin, and myoglobin to denatured metmyoglobin or loss of nitrosylhemochrome.
This suggests that for pork roasts, the state of myoglobin may influence metallic
taste. Additionally, juiciness is reported to decrease with increasing endpoint
temperatures. Therefore, it is possible that moisture content of meats plays
a role in perception of metallic tastes. However, juiciness is shown as orthogonal
to metallic taste, reducing the likelihood of a meaningful relationship between
these attributes. No attempts were reported to isolate the chemical compounds
responsible for the metallic taste.
A ten member panel was used to create a terminology
list useful for the analysis of cooked chicken patties (Lyon, 1987). A PCA was
performed on the terms generated by the panelists over several sessions via
free word association. Metallic was listed as one of the descriptors. Factor
analysis of taste data formed 8 groups of terms, and grouped metallic with terms
like gamey/fowl-like, canned, chemical, and astringent; the last two terms had
low factor loadings and low frequencies of use. Subsequent factor analysis of
the terms remaining after eliminating those with low factor loadings and low
frequencies created 6 factors and this time grouped metallic with liver/organy,
gamey/fowl-like, and canned. Here, canned had low loadings, so it was dismissed
as a useful term. Eventually, the 45 word list initially developed was reduced
to 12 terms. Metallic was the only term not associated with taste or smell,
but instead defined as a feeling factor on the tongue.
Perception of metallic attributes resulting from medication
Research has shown that drugs, including antiretroviral drugs,
are secreted into saliva (Mucklow et al, 1978) at levels high enough to exert
adverse effects on taste sensation by either modifying taste transduction mechanisms
or producing a taste of their own. Many drugs are converted to active metabolites
by intracellular enzymes, but their volumes of distributions are high, which
suggests that the unmetabolized drug is dispersed into extravascular spaces
(Physicians Desk Ref, 1997).
In a study of eighteen adult male outpatients with AIDS Glover et al (1995),
sixteen patients reported a metallic taste resulting from intravenous administration
of pentamidine isethionate. This taste arises independent of eating, but may
be masked or magnified during eating. Following administration, pentamidine
isethionate is distributed throughout the body with a mean elimination half-life
of 29 hours in patients with normal renal function. Sweet foods were reported
to make the unpleasant taste better, while milk and tap water were reported
to make the taste worse. Milk shakes and ice cream, being sweet, were reported
to make the unpleasant tastes decrease. Metallic taste persisted 24 hours a
day from day 6-21, at times leaving patients hungry but unable to eat.
Ritonavir acts as an inhibitor of HIV protease
with activity against the Human Immunodeficiency Virus, HIV, and is also reported
to have a bitter metallic taste (FDA Web Document G, 2000). In contrast to pentamidine
isethionate, ritonavir is practically insoluble in water. Didanosine and lamivudine
are nucleoside analogs also used to treat HIV. Didanosine is described primarily
as bitter, metallic, sour, and medicinal, while lamivudine is described as bitter,
metallic, medicinal, astringent, sour (Schiffman, 1999).
Histamine dihydrochloride is used with interleukin-2
to treat adults with advanced metastatic melanoma that has metastasized to the
liver. Metallic taste is given as a common adverse reaction to taking this medication
(FDA Web Document I, 2000). Amifostine is a thiol compound with wide cytoprotective
activity. Genvresse et al (2001) evaluated amifostine use in the elderly. This
study compared side effects related to doses of 740 mg m-2 in combination with
chemotherapy given to patients 70 years and older, as well as with younger patients
to determine if age has an affect on the tolerability of amifostine. Between
4-5% of the patients reported metallic dry mouth as a side effect of the drug.
Ivancev et al (1989) studied the effect of an
iodinated lipid emulsion in 16 patients for computed tomography of the liver.
Fifteen of the sixteen patients suffered from malignant lesions, while only
one suffered from focal nodular disorder. Four of the patients, suffering from
malignant lesions, described a metallic off-taste as moderate and lasting only
ten minutes. The only patient tested with focal nodular hyperplasia reported
the metallic taste as well, and described it as disturbing and lasting beyond
the end of the 24-hour study. This patient had uncharacteristically low iodinated
lipid emulsion uptake into the liver and high uptake into the spleen when compared
to the 15 patients who suffered from malignant lesions. Iodine containing compounds
have been shown to cause metallic tastes in other contexts. Potassium iodide
can be used to help prevent radioactive iodine uptake in the event of a radiation
emergency (FDA Web Document D, 2001).
Gelenberg et al (1989) report studies comparing
standard and low serum levels of lithium dosages for treatment of bipolar disorder.
They show that standard doses, adjusted to achieve serum lithium concentrations
of 0.8 - 1.0 mmol per liter, were more effective at treating bipolar disorder
than low doses, adjusted to achieve blood serum lithium concentrations of 0.4
-0.6 mmol per liter. However, side effects were reported more frequently in
the standard dose group than in the low dose group. Interestingly, metallic
taste was reported only in the standard dose group, with roughly 20% of patients
receiving therapy reporting a persistent metallic taste after six months of
continuous administration.
Metronidazole is the most extensively studied antibacterial
treatment for inflammatory bowel disease, is quickly absorbed and distributed
throughout the body. MetroLotion is a metronidazole containing lotion that acts
as an antiprotozoal and antibacterial agent (FDA Web Document F, 1998). A metallic
taste is one of the most commonly reported adverse effects with topical use
of metronidazole. Aminosalicylates, corticosteroids, and immunomodulators also
used to treat inflammatory bowel disease report adverse effects, but the presence
of a metallic taste is not listed as a side effect of these treatements (Stein
and Hanauer, 2000). Reports exist showing that metallic taste has occasionally
been reported after intravenous administration of higher doses of clindamycin
phosphate, a drug used in the treatment of infections caused by anaerobic bacteria
(FDA Web Document C, 1998).
Another topical agent that causes a metallic taste
as an adverse reaction is Clinoril. This is a non-steroidal anti-inflammatory
drug that also has utility as an analgesic and antipyretic. (FDA Web Document
J, 2001). This drug is used for many arthritic and inflamed muscle conditions.
Metformin decreases the absorption of vitamin B12 and folic acid. Melchior and
Jaber (1996) discuss the utility of metformin as an antihyperglycemic agent
to treat type II diabetes. In the absence of insulin, metformin has no effect.
Metallic taste is one of the most frequently reported side effects of metformin
used in this context. Glucophage tablets are another form of metformin, composed
of metformin hydrochloride, used in the treatment of type II diabetes and is
freely soluble in water. There are reports among about 3% of users that these
tablets have an unpleasant metallic taste that lasts for a short time when starting
this medication (FDA Web Document H, 2001).
Early symptoms of excessive levels of blood calcium
include a metallic taste in the mouth. Hectoral, used to treat lower high parathyroid
hormone levels in patients undergoing chronic kidney dialysis, affects calcium
absorption in the body (FDA Web Document A, 2002). Rocaltrol is a synthetic
vitamin D analog that helps regulate absorption of calcium in the gastrointestinal
tract (FDA Web Document E, 1998). Calcitriol occurs naturally in humans, 99.9%
bound in blood. Excessive dosages of Rocaltrol can lead to hypercalcemia, leading
to a characteristic metallic taste. The symptoms of Rocaltrol are similar to
those encountered in cases of vitamin D intoxication.
An interesting interaction has been shown to occur
for smokers using silver actetate as a deterrent to smoking. The silver interacts
with smoke from cigarettes to produce an adverse metallic taste that helps aid
in reducing the desire to smoke. Malcolm et al (1986) report evidence of silver
acetate solutions being used as a deterrent to smoking since before 1940. They
performed a study with one thousand subjects given either silver acetate gum
or a placebo to help act as a deterrent to smoking. Statistical differences
existed between the control and experimental conditions. Hymowitz and Eckholdt
(1996) later studied the effect of silver acetate in lozenge form. Five hundred
adult smokers were assigned to either receive a 2.5 mg silver acetate lozenge
or a placebo. However, they report that some smokers did not consider the reaction
to be very aversive, whereas some others considered the reaction to be very
aversive, suggesting different sensitivities may exist among people.
References:
Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry, US Public Health Services.
(1997) ATSDR's Toxicological Profiles: Zinc. Boca Raton, FL: Lewis Publishers,
CRC Press.
ASTM (1991) Standard Practice E679. Determination of odor and taste thresholds by a forced choice ascending concentration series method of limits. American Society for Testing materials, Philadelphia.
Barceloux, D.G. (1999) Zinc. Journal of Toxicology and Clinical Toxicology, 37 (2): 279-92.
Bartoshuk, L. M. (1978) "History of taste research" in Handbook of Perception (Carterette, E.C., Friedman, M. P., eds), Academic Press, pp. 3-18.
Bartoshuk, L.M. (1993) The biological basis of food perception and acceptance. Food Quality and Preference. 4, 21-32.
Bartoshuk, L.M., Kveton, J. (1991) Peripheral source of taste phantom demonstrated by topical anesthesia. Chemical Senses, 16, 499-500.
Berry, B.W., Maga, J.A., Calkins, C.R., Wells, L.H., Carpenter, A.L., Cross, H.R. (1980) Flavor profile analysis of cooked beef loin steaks. Journal of Food Science, 45: 1113-1115, 1121.
Blouin, F.A., Vinnet, C.H. (1985) Taste/flavor characteristics of edible cottonseed flour. Paper No. 85, Abstracts 190th American Chemical Society National Meeting, Sept 8-13. Dib. Agricultural and Food Chemistry, Chicago, IL.
Bonvehí, J.S., Coll, F.V. (2000) Evaluation of purine alkaloids and diketopiperazines contents in processed cocoa powder. Euro Food Res. Techno, 210, 189-195.
Böröcz-Szabó, M. (1980) The influence of iron contamination on the sensory properties of liquid foods. Acta alimentaria, 9 (4) 341-356.
Boudreau, J. C. (1986) Neurophysiology and Human Taste Sensations. Journal of Sensory Studies. 1, 185-202.
Boudreau, J. C., Oravec, J., Hoang, N.K., White, T.D. (1979) Taste and the taste of foods. Ch. 1 in Food Taste Chemistry (J.C. Boudreau, ed.) American Chemical Society, Washington, D.C., p. 1.
Boudreau, J.C. (1983) Chemical senses and food flavor: an overview. Ch. 2 in Instrumental Analysis of Foods Vol 1, G. Charlambout and G. Ingeltt, eds, Academic Press, New York, p. 17.
Brand, J. G., Bryant, B.P. (1994) Receptor mechanisms for flavour stimuli. Food Quality and Preference, 5 (1/2) 31-40
Buettner, A., Schieberle, P. (1999) Characterization of the most odor-active volatiles in fresh, hand-squeezed juice of grapefruit. J. Agric. Food Chem, 47: 5189-5193.
Buettner, A., Schieberle, P. (2001) Evaluation of key aroma compounds in hand-squeezed grapefruit juice (Citrus paradisi Macfayden) by quantitation and flavor reconstruction experiments. J. Agric. Food Chem., 49: 1358-1363.
Buscailhon, S., Berdagué, J.L., Bousset, J., Cornet, M., Gandemer, G., Touraille, C., Monin, G. (1994) Relations between compositional traits and sensory qualities of French dry-cured ham. Meat Science, 37: 229-243.
Buttery, R.G., Seifert, R.M., Guadagni, D.G., Ling, L.C. (1971) Characterization of additional volatile components of tomato. J. Agric. Food Chem., 19: 524-529.
Chakraborty, S.; Baruah, A.C. (1971) Kind of water used in tea factories and its effect on quality. Two and a Bud, 18 (1): 25.
Clifford, M.N.; Ohiokpehai, O. (1983) Coffee astringency. Analytical Proceedings, 20 (2): 83-86.
Davidson, S. (1984) Analysing off-flavors. Rural Research, 122:11-14.
Davies, W.L. (1936) The Chemistry of Milk. Chapman and Hall, Ltd. London. P. 387.
De Graaf, C., Legger, A., Roozen, J.P. (1990) J. Sci. Food. Agric. 50, 363-375.
Degouy, E. (1993) The low fat cheese challenge. Dairy Industries International, 58 (10): 41-43.
Delwiche, J. (1996) "Are there 'basic' tastes?" Trends in Food Science and Technology, 7, 411-415.
DuPont, M.S., Muzquiz, M., Estella, I., Genwick, G.R., Price, K.R. (1994) Relationship between sensory properties of lupin seed with alkaloid and tannin content. J. Sci. Food Agric., 65: 95-100.
El Dadek, G.; Moneib, A.; Gharib, S.Y. (1972) Studies on improvement of Kareish cheese. II. Effect of salt and hydrogen ion concentration on the physical and chemical properties of the cheese. Agricultural Research Review, 50 (3): 179-184.
El-Deiry, A., McCabe, B.F. (1990) Temporal lobe tumor manifested by localized dysgeusia. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 99, 586-7.
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document A (1999) Hectoral. (Updated 4/7/00 Revised: 3/15/01) www.fda.gov/cder/consumerinfo/druginfo/hectorol.htm, last accessed April 2002.
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document B (2002) Classification of illnesses attributable to foods. :www.fda.gov/ora/inspect_ref/iom/exhibits/x910c.pdf (last accessed: April 2002).
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document C (1998) Guidance for Industry: Labeling guidance for clindamycin phosphate injection, USP. www.fda.gov/cder/guidance/clindaiv.pdf (last accessed: April 2002).
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document D (2001) Potassium Iodide Recip Product Label. www.fda.gov/ohrms/dockets/dailys/01/Aug01/082801/sup0001.pdf (last accessed: April 2002)
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document E (1998) Rocaltrol www.fda.gov/cder/foi/label/1998/21068lbl.pdf (last accessed: April 2002).
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document F (1998) Physician Package Insert, MetroLotion. www.fda.gov/cder/foi/label/1998/20901lbl.pdf (last accessed: April 2002).
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document G (2000) Novir. www.fda.gov/cder/foi/label/2001/20659s26lbl.pdf (last accessed: April 2002).
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document H (2001) Clinoril. www.fda.gov/cder/foi/label/2001/17911s66lbl.pdf (last accessed: April 2002)
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document H (2001) Glucophage. www.fda.gov/cder/foi/label/2001/20357s22lbl.pdf (last accessed: April 2002).
FDA - Food and Drug Administration, Web Document I (2000) Histamine dihydrochloride www.fda.gov/ohrms/dockets/ac/00/backgrd/3671b1_07.pdf (last accessed: April 2002).
Frank, J. (1984) Improving the flavor of cultured buttermilk. Cultured Dairy Products Journal, 00, 6-9.
Gains, N., Thomson, D.M.H. (1990) Sensory profiling of canned lager beers using consumers in their own homes. Food Quality and Preference, 2, 39-47.
Gautschi, M., Schmid, J.P., Peppar, T.L., Ryan, T.P., Tuorto, R.M., Yang, X. (1997) Chemical characterization of diketopiperazines in beer. J. Agric. Food Chem., 45: 3183-3189.
Gelenberg, A.J.; Kane, J.; Keller, M.B.; Lavori, P; Rosenbaum, J.F.; Cole, K.; Lavelle, J. (1989) Comparison of Standard and Low Serum Levels of Lithium for Maintenance Treatment of Bipolar Disorder. The New England Journal of Medicine, 321 (22): 1489-93.
Genvresse, I.; Lange, C.; Schanz, J.; Schweigert, M.; Harder, H.; Possinger, K.; Spath-Schwalbe, E. (2001) Tolerability of the cytoprotective agent amifostine in elderly patients receiving chemothreapy: a comparative study. Anti-Cancer Drugs, 12 (4): 345-9.
Glover, J.; Dibble, S.; Miaskowski, C.; Geibert, R. (1995) Changes in taste associated with intravenous administration of pentamidine. Journal of the Associated Nurses of AIDS Care, 6 (1): 43-8.
Gonázlez Viñas, M.A., Salvador, M.D. (1998) Comparison of two simple methods for the measurement of detection thresholds for basic, umami, and metallic tastes. J. Sensory Studies. 13, 299-314.
Guth, H., Grosch, W. (1989) 3-Methylnonane-2-4-dione - An intense odor compound formed during flavour reversion of soya-bean oil. Fat Sciences Technologies, 93:225-230.
Hanson, N.A.; Duckworth, H.A.G. (1982) CAP = preservation. Packaging, 53 (625): 33.
Hatakanaka, A. (1996) The fresh green odor emitted by plants. Food Rev. Int., 12: 303-350.
Hatanaka, A., Kajiwara, T., Harada, T. (1975) Biosynthetic pathway of cucumber alcohol: transt-2-cis-6-nonadienol via cis-3,cis-6-nonadienal. Phytochemistry, 14, 2589-2592.
Heiler, C., Schieberle, P. (1997) Quantitative instrumental and sensory studies on aroma compounds contributing to a metallic flavour defect in buttermilk. International Dairy Journal, 7, 659-666.
Henning, H. (1916) Die qualitatenrihe des geschmacks. Z. Pscyhol., 203 - 219.
Heymann, H., Hedrick, H.B., Karrasch, M.A., Eggeman, M.K., Ellersieck, M.R. (1990) Sensory and chemical characteristics of fresh pork roasts cooked to different endpoint temperatures. J. Food Sci., 55 (3): 613-617.
Hymowitz, N., Eckholdt, H. (1996) Effects of a 2.5 mg silver acetate lozenge on initial and long term smoking cessation. Preventative Medicine, 24: 537-546.
International Labour Office (1983) Encyclopedia of occupational and health safety, Vol. I and II, 3rd ed.
ISO (1991) Sensory analysis. Methodology. Method of investigating sensitivity of taste. ISO Standard 3972.
Ivancevik, K.; Lunderquist, A.; Isaksson, A.; Hochberfs, P.; Wretlind, A. (1989) Clinical trials with a new iodinated lipid emulsion for computed topography of the liver. Acta Radiologica, 30(5): 449-57.
Jeremiah, L.E., Ball, R.O., Uttaro, B., Gibson, L.L. (1996) The relationship of chemical components to flavor attributes of bacon and ham. Food Research International, 29 (5/6): 4457-464.
Johnsen, P.B., Civille, G.V., Cervellotti, J.R. (1987) A lexicon of pond-raised catfish flavor descriptors. J. Sens. Stud., 2: 85-91.
Kachru, D.N., Tandon, S.K., Misra, U.K., Nag, D. (1989) Occupational lead poisoning among silver jewellery workers. Indian Journal of Medical Sciences, 43 (4):89-91.
Kim, T.H., Nguyen, T.T., Shin, J.H., Baek, H.H., Lee, H.J. (2000) Aroma-active compounds of miniature beefsteakplant (Mosla diantherea Maxim). J. Agric. Food Chem., 48: 2877-2881.
Kinnamon, S. C., Cummings, T. A. (1992) "Chemosensory transduction mechanisms in taste." Annual Review of Physiology, 54, 715-731.
Kumazawa, K., Masuda, H. (1999) Identification of potent odorants in Japanese green tea (Sen-cha). J. Agric. Food Chem, 47: 5169-5172.
Lindner, K., Czétényi, C., Kubát, K., Bolla, É., Szejtli, J. (1977) Sensory evaluation of a new dihyrochalcone sweetener: Part II - Studies in foods. Acta Alimnetaria, 6 (4): 323-330.
Linssen, J.P.H.; Janssens, J.L.G.M.; Roozen, J.P.; Posthumus, M.A. (1991) Sensory descriptors for a taint in water packed in test pouches made of polyethylene lined aluminum. Journal of Plastic Film and Sheeting, 7 (4): 294-305.
Lovell, R.T. (1983) Off-flavors in pond-cultured channel catfish. Wat. Scie. Tech, 15: 67-73.
Lundahl, D.S., Lukes, M. R., McDaniel, M. R., Henderson, L.A. (1986) A semiascending paired difference method for determining sensory threshold of added substances to background media. J. Sensory Studies, 1, 291-306.
Lyon, B.G. (1987) Development of chicken flavor descriptor attribute terms aided by multivariate statistical procedures. Journal of Sensory Studies, 2 (1): 55-67.
MacLeod, G., Coppock, B.M. (1978) Sensory properties of aroma of beef cooked conventionally and by microwave radiation. Journal of Food Science, 43: 145-161.
Mahadeviah, M.; Gowramma, R.V.; Naresh, R. (1983) Welded OTS cans as an alternative to soldered can. Journal of Food Science and Technology - India, 20 (5): 241-242.
Mahaffey, K.R., Corneliussen, P.E., Jelinek, C.F., Fiorino, J.A. (1975) Heavy metal exposure from foods. Environmental Health Perspectives, 12: 63-69.
Malcolm, R.; Currey, H.S.; Mitchell, M.A.; Keil, J.E. (1986) Sliver acetate gum as a deterrent to smoking. Chest, 90 (1): 107-11.
Mallevialle, J., Suffet, I.H. (1987) Identification and treatment of tastes and odors in drinking water. American Water Works Association, Denver, CO.
McBurney, D. H., Gent, J.F. (1979) "On the nature of taste qualities." Psychological Bulletin. 86 (1) 151-167.
Melchoir, W.R.; Jaber, L.A. (1996) Metformin: an antihyperglycemic agent for treatment of type II diabetes. The Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 30 (2): 158-164.
Morgan, B.H., Siu, R.G.H. (1957) Action of ionizing radiations on individual foods. The U.S. Army Research and Development Series No. 1, U.S. Army Quarter-master Corps, Washington.
Mozell, M. M., Smith, P. E., Sullivan, R. L. Jr., Swender, P. (1969) "A technique to occlude the nasal chemoreceptors during lingual flavor stimulation." Physiology and Behavior, 4, 131.
Mucklow, J.C., Bending, M.R., Kahn, G.C., Dollery, C.T. (1978) Drug concentration in saliva. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 24, 563.
Müller, R., Zschiesche, W.; Steffen, H.M.; Schaller, K.H. (1989) Tellurium - intoxication. Klin Wochenschr, 67: 1152-1155.
Nakagawa, M. (1972) Relationship between astringency and the reaction of astringents with proteins. Journal of the Japanese Society for Food Science and Technology, 19 (11): 531-537.
Norgen, R., Pfaffman, C. (1975) The pontine taste area in the rat. Brain Research. 91, 99-117.
Persson, T., von Sydow, E., Akesson, C. (1973) Aroma of canned beef: sensory properties. Journal of Food Science, 38: 386-392.
Physician's Desk Reference (1997) 51st ed. Montvale, NJ: Medical Economics Company.
Plattig, K. H. (1988) "The Sense of Taste" in Sensory Analysis of Food (Piggott, J.R., ed.). Elsevier Science Publishers, Ltd. London, Great Britain, pp. 1-24.
Poretta, S. (1995) Quality evaluation of canned whole tomatoes. Food Science and Technology International, 1: 97-104.
Portmann, M-O., Kilcast, D. (1996) Psychophysical characterization of new sweeteners of commercial importance for the EC food industry. Food Chemistry. 56 (3) 291-302.
Pritchard, T. C. (1991) "The primate gustatory systems" in Smell and Taste in Health and Disease (Getchell, T. V., Bartoshuk, L. M., Doty, R. L., and Snow, J.B. Jr., eds.), Raven Press, pp. 109-125.
Refsgaard, H.H.F., Brockoff, P.B., Jensen, B. (1998) Sensory and chemical changes in farmed Atlantic salmon. J. Agric. Food Chem., 46: 3473-3479.
Refsgaard, H.H.F., Brockoff, P.B., Jensen, B. (2000) Free polyunsaturated fatty acids cause taste deterioration of salmon during frozen storage. J. Agric. Food Chem, 48: 3280-3285.
Rice, W.H., McMahon, D.J. (1998) Chemical, physical, and sensory characteristics of Mozzarella cheese fortified using protein chelated iron or ferric chloride. J. Dairy Sci., 81: 318-326.
Risvik, E. (1986) Sensory evaluation of irradiated beef and bacon. J. of Sens. Studies, 1: 109-122.
Roper, S.D. (1989) The cell biology of vertebrate taste receptors. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 329-353.
Roy, G. (1992) A review of sweet taste potentiation brought about by divalent oxygen and sulfur incorporation. Crit. Rev. in Food Sci. and Nut., 31 (1/2): 59-77.
Rychlik, M., Schieberle, P., Grosch, W. (1998) Compilation of odor thresholds, odor qualities, and retention indices of key food odorants. Deutsche Forschungsanstalt feur Lebensmittelchemie: Garching, Germany.
Schiffman, S. S., Erickson, R. P. (1971) "A theoretical review: A psychophysical model for gustatory quality." Physiology and Behavior. 7, 617-633.
Schiffman, S. S.; Erickson, R. P. (1980) "The issue of primary tastes versus a taste continuum." Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 4: 109-117.
Schiffman, S.S., Zervakis, J., Shaio, E., Heald, A.E. (1999) Effect of the nucleoside analogs zidovudine, didanosine, stavudine, and lamivudine on the sense of taste. Nutrition, 15 (11/12) pp. 854-859.
Shewfelt, R.L. (1981) Fish muscle lipolysis - a review. J. Food Biochem, 5: 79-100.
Shrader, J.H., Johnson, A.H. (1934) Freesing orange juice. Ind. Engng Chem, 26, 869-874.
Skramlick, E. von. (1926) Handbuch der physiologie der niederen sinne. Leipzig, Germany. Georg Threme, ed.
Smith-Swintosky, V. L., Plata-Salaman, C. R., Scott, T. R. (1991) "Gustatory neural coding in the monkey cortex: Stimulus quality." Journal of Neurophysiology. 66, 1156-1165.
Stein, R.B., Hanauer, S.B. (2000) Comparative tolerability of treatments for inflammatory bowel disease. Drug Safefy, 23 (5): 429-448.
Stephan, A., Steinhart, H. (1999) Identification of character impact odorants of different soybean lecithins. J. Agric. Food Chem, 47: 2854-2859.
Suffet, I.H.; Corado, A.; Chou, D.; McGuire, M.J.; Butterworth, S. (1996) AWWA taste and odor survey. Journal of American Water Works Association, 88 (4): 168-180.
Whitfield, F.B., Freeman, D.J., Last, K.H., Bannister, P.A., Kennett, B.H. (1982) Oct-1-en-3-ol and (5Z)-octa-1,5-dien-3-ol, compounds important in the flavour of prawns and sand-lobsters. Australian Journal of Chemistry, 35: 373-83.
Yanagisawa, K., Bartoshuk, L.M., Catalanotto, F.A., Lehman, C.D., Karrer, T.A., Kveton, J.F. (1998) Anestheisa of the chorda tympani nerve and taste phantoms. Physiology and Behavior, 63 (3) 329-335.
Yanagisawa, K., Bartoshuk, L.M., Karrer, T.A., Kveton, J.F., Catalanotto, F.A., Lehman, C.D., Weiffenbach, J.M. (1992) Anesthesia of the chorda tympani nerve: insights into a source of dysgeusia. Chemical Senses, 17, 724.
Yau, N.J.N., Huang, Y.J. (2000) The effect of membrane processed water on sensory
properties of Oolong tea drinks. Food Quality and Preference, 11: 331-339.